f The New Science of Happiness

The New Science of Happiness

The New Science of Happiness

By Claudia Wallis

What makes the human heart sing? Researchers are taking a close look. What they’ve found may surprise you!

SUGARY WHITE SAND GLEAMS UNDER THE BRIGHT YUCATAN SUN, AQUAMARINE water teems with tropical fish and lazy sea turtles, cold Mexican beer beckons beneath the shady thatch of palapas-it’s hard to imagine a sweeter spot than Akumal, Mexico, to contemplate the joys of being alive. And that was precisely the agenda when three leading psychologists met up in this Mexican paradise to plot a new direction for psychology.

For most of its history, psychology had concerned itself with all that ails the human mind: anxiety, depression, neurosis, obsessions, paranoia, delusions. The goal of practitioners was to bring patients from a negative, ailing state to a neutral normal, or, as University of Pennsylvania psychologist Martin Seligman puts it, “from a minus five to a zero.” It was Seligman who had summoned the others to Akumal that New year’s Day in 1998 – his first day as president of the American Psychologist Association (A.P.A)-to share a vision of a new goal for psychology. “I realized that my profession was half-baked. It wasn’t enough for us to nullify disabling conditions and get to zero. We need to ask, What are the enabling conditions that make human beings flourish? How do we get from zero to plus five?”

Every incoming A.P.A. president is asked to choose a theme for his or her yearlong term in office. Seligman was thinking big. He wanted to persuade substantial numbers in the profession to explore the region north of zero, to look at what actively made people feel fulfilled, engaged and meaningfully happy. Mental health, he reasoned, should be more than the absence of mental illness. It should be something akin to a vibrant and muscular fitness of the human mind and spirit.

Over the decade, a few psychological researchers had ventured out of the dark realm of mental illness into the sunny land of the mentally hale and hearty. Some of Seligman’s own research, for instance, had focused on optimism, a trait shown to be associated with good physical health, less depression and mental illness, longer life and, yes, greater happiness. Perhaps the most eager explorer of this terrain was University of Illinois psychologist Edward Diener, a.k.a. Dr. Happiness. For more than two decades Diener had been examining what does and does not make people feel satisfied with life. Seligman’s goal was to shine a light on such work and encourage much, much more of it.

To help him realize his vision, Seligman invited Ray Fowler, then the long-reigning and influential CEO of the A.P.A., to join him in Akumal. He also invited Hungarian-born psychologist Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi (pronounced Cheeks-sent-me-high), best known for exploring a happy state of mind called flow, the feeling of complete engagement in a creative or playful activity familiar to athletes, musicians, video-game enthusiasts – almost anyone who loses himself in a favorite pursuit. By the end of their week at the beach, the three had plans for the first-ever conference on positive psychology, to be held in Akumal a year later. It was to become an annual event.

[adrotate group=”1″]

Within a few months, Seligman, who has a talent for popularizing and promoting his areas of interest, was approached by the Templeton Foundation, which proceeded to create lucrative awards for research in positive psych. The result: an explosion of research on happiness, optimism, positive emotions and health character traits. Seldom has an academic field been brought so quickly and deliberately to life.

What Makes Us Happy

So, what has science learned about what makes the human heart sing? More than one might imagine – along with some surprising things about what doesn’t ring our inner chimes. Take wealth, for instance, and all the delightful things that money can buy. Research has shown that once your basic needs are met, additional income does little to raise your sense of satisfaction with life. A good education? Sorry, Mom and Dad, neither education nor, for that matter, a high IQ paves the road to happiness. Youth? No, again. In fact, older people are more consistently satisfied with their live than the young. And they’re less prone to dark moods: a recent survey by the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention found that people aged between 20 and 24 are sad for an average of 3.4 days a month, as opposed to just 2.3 days for people ages 65 to 74. Marriage? A complicated picture: married people are generally happier than singles, but that may be because they were happier to begin with. Watching TV? Not at all. People who watch more than three hours a day – especially soaps – are more unhappy than those who spend less time in front of the box.

On the positive side, religious faith seems genuinely to lift the spirit, though it’s tough to tell whether it’s the God part or the community aspect that does the heavy lifting. Friends? A giant yes. A 2002 study conducted at the University of Illinois by Diener and Seligman found that the most salient characteristics shared by the 10% of students with the highest levels of happiness and the fewest signs of depression were their strong ties to friends and family, and commitment to spending time with them. “It’s important to work on social skills, close interpersonal ties and social support in order to be happy,”says Diener.

Raut Veenhoven, professor of happiness studies at Erasmus University in Rotterdam, is a font of such happy facts. He edits the Journal of Happiness Studies and manages the World Database of Happiness (www2.eur.nl/fsw/research/happiness), a massive archive of research gleaned from all over the world. Clicking around on the database in his rather dreary high-rise office, Veenhoven notes that people who drink one or two glasses of alcohol a day are happier than teetotallers; people between the ages of 30 and 50 are less happy than other groups perhaps, he says, because in mid-life we have “less freedom and more responsibilities” in terms of kids, jobs and mortgages; and people are happiest in jobs that afford a certain amount of freedom and decision-making power. After working in the field for 25 years, Veenhoven has concluded that happiness is “how much you like the life you are living. People can live in paradise and still be unhappy because they screw up everything in their lives.”

Measuring Our Moods

Of course, happiness is not a static state. Even the happiest of people-the cheeriest 10%-feel blue at times. And even the bluest have their moments of joy. That has presented a challenge to social scientists trying to measure happiness. To get around those challenges, researchers have devised several methods of assessment. Diener has created one of the most basic and widely used tools, the Satisfaction With Life Scale. Though some scholars have questioned the validity of this simple, five-question survey, Diener has found that it squares well with other measures of happiness, such as impressions from friends and family, expression of positive emotion and low incidence of depression.

Researchers have devised other tools to look at more transient moods. Csikszentmihalyi pioneered a method of using beepers and, later, handheld computers to contact subjects at random intervals. A pop-up screen presents an array of questions: What are you doing? How much are you enjoying it? Are you alone or interacting with someone else? The method, called experience sampling, but it provides an excellent picture of satisfaction and engagement at a specific time during a specific activity.

In December, a team lead by Nobel-prizewinning psychologist Daniel Kahneman of Princeton University unveiled a new tool for sizing up happiness: they day-reconstruction method. Participants fill out a long diary and questionnaire detailing everything they did on the previous day and whom they were with at the time and rating a range of feelings during each episode (happy, impatient, depressed, worried, tired and so on) on a seven-point scale. His studies show that what you remember of an experience is particularly influenced by the emotional high and low points and by how it ends. So, if you were randomly to beep someone on vacation in Italy, you might catch that person waiting furiously for a slow-moving waiter to take an order or grousing about the high cost of pottery. But if you ask when it’s over, “How was the vacation in Italy?” the average person remembers the peak moments and how he or she felt at the end of the trip. Kahneman therefore believes that social scientists studying happiness should pay careful attention to people’s actual experiences rather than just survey their reflections. That, he feels, is especially relevant if research is to inform quality-of-life policies like how much money our society should devote to parks and recreation, or how much should be invested in improving worker’s commutes, “You cannot ignore how people spend their time,” he says, “when thinking about well-being.”

Seligman, in contrast puts the emphasis on the remembering self. “I think we are our memories more than we are the sum total of our experiences,” he says. For him, studying moment-to-moment experiences puts too much emphasis on transient pleasures and displeasure. Happiness goes deeper than that, he argues in his 2002 book Authentic Happiness. As a result of his research, he finds three components of happiness: pleasure (“the smiley-face piece”), engagement (the depth of involvement with one’s family, work, romance and hobbies) and meaning (using personal strengths to serve some larger end). Of those three roads to a happy, satisfied life, pleasure is the least consequential, he insists: “This is newsworthy because so many people build their lives around pursuing pleasure.”

[adrotate group=”1″]

Can We Get Happier?

One of the biggest issues in happiness research is the question of how much our happiness is under our control. In 1996 University of Minnesota researcher David Lykken published a paper looking at the role of genes in determining one’s sense of satisfaction in life. Lykken, now 76, gathered information on 4,000 sets of twins born in Minnesota from 1936 through 1955. After comparing happiness data on identical vs. fraternal twins, he came to the conclusion that about 50% of one’s satisfaction with life comes from genetic programming. (Genes influence such traits as having a sunny, easygoing personality; dealing well with stress; and feeling low levels of anxiety and depression.) Lykken found that circumstantial factors like income, marital status, religion and education contribute only about 8% to one’s overall well-being. He attributes the remaining percentage to “life’s slings and arrows.”

According to Dylan Evans, of the University of the West England in Bristol and author of Emotion: The Science of Sentiment, our happiness has been determined by the exigencies of evolution. “Those things that would have helped our ancestors to survive and reproduce-romantic relationships, parental relationships with children, close friendships – tend to bring us the most happiness,” he points out. Indeed, the primary role of happiness, Evans argues, is to motivate us to want to spread our happiness, and our genes, around: “If evolution set up happiness as an internal carrot, it set up sadness as a stick to goad us to do the things we need to do to preserve and perpetuate our genes.”

Because of the large influence of our genes, Lykken proposed the idea that each of us has a happiness set point much like our set point for body weight. No matter what happens in our life-good, bad, spectacular, horrific-we tend to return in short order to our set range. Some post-tsunami images of smiling Asian children returning to school underscored this amazing capacity we have to right ourselves.

A substantial body of research documents our tendency to return to the norm. A study of lottery winners done in 1978 found, for instance, that they did not wind up significantly happier than a control group. Even people who lose the use of their limbs to a devastating accident tend to bounce back, through perhaps not all the way to their base line. One study found that a week after the accident, the injured were severely angry and anxious, but after eight weeks, “happiness was their strongest emotion,” says Diener. “Everyone is surprised by how happy paraplegics can be,” Kahneman. “The reason is that they are not paraplegic full time. They do other things. They enjoy their meals, their friends. They read the news. It has to do with the allocation of attention.”

Peter Herschbach, a psychology professor at Munich’s Technical University, found a similar resilience among breast-cancer patients. In a study of 385 women undergoing treatment for the disease, Herschbach found that more than 50% reported that they had lived “more intensively and more consciously” as a result of their ordeal and developed better relationships with their partners. “Patients facing a long, uncertain course of treatment continuously adapt their values to their worsening health status,” he says. “They are thus able to preserve an amazingly high level of contentedness.”

Edward Diener has found two life events that seem to knock people lastingly below their happiness set point: loos of a spouse and loss of a job. It takes five to eight years for a widow to regain her previous sense of well-being. Similarly, the effects of a job loss linger long after the individual has return to the work force.

[adrotate group=”1″]

When he proposed his set-point theory eight years ago, Lykken came to a drastic conclusion. “It may be that trying to be happier is as futile as trying to be taller,” he wrote. He has since come to regret that sentence. “I made a dumb statement in the original article,” he says. “It’s clear that we can change our happiness levels widely – up or down.”

Lykken’s revisionist thinking coincides with the view of the positive-psychology movement, which has put a premium on research showing you can raise your level of happiness. For Seligman and like-minded researchers, that involves working on the three components of happiness-getting more pleasure out of life (which can be done by savoring sensory experiences, although, he warns, “you’re never going to make a curmudgeon into a giggly person”), becoming more engaged in what you do and finding ways of making your life feel more meaningful.

There are numerous ways to do that, they argue. At the University of California at Riverside, psychologist Sonja Lyubomirsky is studying different kinds of happiness boosters. One is the “gratitude journal”-a diary in which subjects write down things for which they are thankful. She has found that taking the time to count their blessings conscientiously once a week significantly increased subject’s overall satisfaction with life over a period of six weeks, whereas a control group that did not keep journals had no such gain.

Gratitude exercises can do more than lift ones mood. At the University of California at Davis, psychologist Robert Emmons found they improve physical health, raise energy levels and, for patients with neuromuscular disease, relieve pain and fatigue. “The ones who benefited more tended to elaborate more and have a wider span of things they’re grateful for,” he notes.

Another happiness booster, say positive psychologists, is performing acts of altruism or kindness-visiting a nursing home, helping a friend’s child with homework, mowing a neighbor’s lawn, writing a letter to a grandparent. Doing five kind acts a week, especially all in a single day, gave a measurable boost to Lyubomirsky’s subjects.

Seligman has tested similar interventions in controlled trials at Penn and in huge experiments conducted over the internet. The single most effective way to turborcharge your joy, he says, is to make a “gratitude visit.”That means writing a testimonial thanking a teacher, pastor or grandparent-anyone to whom you owe a debt of gratitude-and then visiting that person to read him or her the letter of appreciation. “The remarkable thing,” says Seligman, “is that people who do this just once are measurably happier and less depressed a month later. But it’s gone by three months.” Less powerful but more lasting, he says, is taking time each day to write down a trio of things that went well and why.

The value of connecting with other people seems to be the most fundamental finding from the science of happiness. “Almost every person feels happier when they’re with other people,” observes Csikszentmihalyi. “It’s paradoxical because many of us think we can hardly wait to get home and be alone with nothing to do, but that’s a worst-case scenario. If you’re alone with nothing to do, the quality of your experience really plummets.”But can a loner really become more gregarious through acts-of-kindness exercises? Can a dyed-in-the-wool pessimist learn to see the glass as half full? Can gratitude journals work their magic over the long haul? And how many of us could keep filling them with fresh thankful thoughts year after year? Lyubomirsky believe it’s all possible: “Every day you have to renew your commitment. Hopefully, some of the strategies will become habitual over time and not a huge effort.”

But other psychologists are more skeptical. Some simply doubt that personality is that flexible or that individuals can or should change their habitual coping styles. “If you’re a pessimist who really thinks through in detail what might go wrong, that’s a strategy that’s likely to work very well for you, “says Julie Norem, a psychology professor at Wellesley College, Massachusetts, and the author of The Positive Power of Negative Thinking. “In fact, you may be messed up if you try to substitute a positive attitude.” She is worried that the messages of positive psychology reinforce “a lot of biases” about how individual initiative and a positive attitude can solve complex problems.

Who’s right? This is an experiment we can all do for ourselves. There’s little risk in trying some extra gratitude and kindness, and the results-should they materialize are their own reward.